% IMPORTANT: The following is UTF-8 encoded. This means that in the presence
% of non-ASCII characters, it will not work with BibTeX 0.99 or older.
% Instead, you should use an up-to-date BibTeX implementation like “bibtex8” or
% “biber”.
@INPROCEEDINGS{Heckner:1010403,
author = {Heckner, Marisa and Cieslik, Edna and Paas Oliveros, Lya
Katarina and Eickhoff, Simon and Patil, Kaustubh and
Langner, Robert},
title = {{P}redicting {E}xecutive {F}unctioning from {B}rain
{N}etworks: {M}odality {S}pecificity and {A}ge {E}ffects},
reportid = {FZJ-2023-03043},
year = {2023},
note = {Acknowledgements: This study was supported by the Deutsche
Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, EI 816/11-1, PA 3634/1-1, EI
816/21-1, SPP2041, CRC), the National Institute of Mental
Health (R01-MH074457), the Helmholtz Portfolio Theme
“Supercomputing and Modeling for the Human Brain”, the
European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation
Programme under Grant Agreement No. 720270 (HBP SGA1),
785907 (HBP SGA2), and 945539 (HBP SGA3).},
abstract = {Healthy aging is associated with structural and functional
brain changes, which have beenlinked to executive
functioning (EF) decline. Despite a known association
between resting-statefunctional connectivity (RSFC) and EF
[1], its potential for capturing individual differences inEF
performance has been questioned [2]. Therefore, we examined
to what degree individual EFabilities may be predicted from
different brain modalities: RSFC, grey-matter volume
(GMV),regional homogeneity (ReHo), and fractional amplitude
of low frequency fluctuations (fALFF).This was done in an
EF-associated (EFN), a perceptuomotor (PercMot) and a
whole-brainnetwork in young and old adults.We
meta-analytically defined an EFN [3] and a perceptuomotor
(linked to visual,auditory, and motor processing) network
[4] and used Power et al.’s [5] graph of
putativefunctional areas as a whole-brain control. Imaging
and behavioral data of 116 younger (age: 20–40 years, 64
females) and 111 older (age: 60–80 years, 72 females)
healthy adults were obtainedfrom the enhanced NKI sample
[6]. Targets for prediction comprised performance in
highlydemanding (HD) and less demanding (LD) conditions of
each of 3 classic EF tasks: Color-WordInterference, Trail
Making, and N-Back. Individual z-transformed performance
scores were thenpredicted from the characteristics of each
network’s edges (RSFC) or nodes (GMV, ReHo,fALFF) using
partial least-squares regression with 100 repetitions of a
10-fold cross-validationscheme. Prediction accuracy as
indicated by the root mean square error (RMSE) for the
100repetitions was then submitted to a 2(age group) ×
3(network) × 2(task demand level) ×4(modalities)
mixed-measures ANOVA.Overall, prediction accuracy (i.e.,
RMSE) was rather low to moderate. Similar toprevious studies
[2;7], prediction accuracy was better for older [M = .784]
(vs. younger [M =.794]) adults, indicating that
brain–behavior association strength increases with
advancing age.This might be due to overall age-related
neural decline such as atrophy or white-matterdegeneration,
changes that have been associated with network integrity [8]
and reorganization aswell as EF decline. Interestingly,
prediction accuracy for younger adults was better
whenpredicting HD [M = .757] (vs. LD [M = .831]) conditions,
while for older adults, predictionaccuracy was better when
predicting LD [M = .771] (vs. HD [M = .797]) conditions.
Possibly,age-related effects on the network level might
still be compensated for in LD by some form ofcompensation
such as the additional recruitment of domain-general
resources [9] subserved bythe networks investigated here,
but not in HD conditions. Prediction accuracy for younger
adultswas best with fALFF [M = .751] as compared to the
other modalities [MGMV = .804, MRSFC =.802, MReHo = .818],
while for older adults, the highest accuracy was achieved
with GMV [M =.754] as compared to the other modalities
[MfALFF = .783, MRSFC = .795, MReHo = .803]. GMV isstrongly
linked to atrophy and its pattern of decline is thought to
be rather consistent across olderadults [10]. Together with
an age-related decrease in performance, this suggests that
GMV is amore sensitive marker for individual EF abilities in
older adults. For younger adults, fALFF, ameasure of
functional variability, might be the more sensitive marker,
possibly mediated via itslink to cognitive flexibility
[11].Summing up, our results show modality- as well as
demand-level specificity. However,overall low
brain–behavior associations as well as the missing network
specificity, question thepotential of the applied single
metrics as markers for individual differences in EF
performance.Rather, our findings suggest that future
research may need to analyze more global properties ofthe
brain, possibly combining different structural and
functional metrics or task states as well asage-adapted
behavioral testing to result in sensitive predictors for
young and old adults,respectively.},
month = {Jul},
date = {2023-07-22},
organization = {Organization for Human Brain Mapping
(OHBM), Montreal (Canada), 22 Jul 2023
- 26 Jul 2023},
subtyp = {After Call},
cin = {INM-7},
cid = {I:(DE-Juel1)INM-7-20090406},
pnm = {5252 - Brain Dysfunction and Plasticity (POF4-525) / JL
SMHB - Joint Lab Supercomputing and Modeling for the Human
Brain (JL SMHB-2021-2027)},
pid = {G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5252 / G:(DE-Juel1)JL SMHB-2021-2027},
typ = {PUB:(DE-HGF)24},
doi = {10.34734/FZJ-2023-03043},
url = {https://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/1010403},
}