001     1037612
005     20250203124522.0
024 7 _ |a 10.1017/langcog.2023.21
|2 doi
024 7 _ |a 1866-9808
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a 1866-9859
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a 10.34734/FZJ-2025-00783
|2 datacite_doi
024 7 _ |a WOS:001009572600001
|2 WOS
037 _ _ |a FZJ-2025-00783
082 _ _ |a 400
100 1 _ |a Wehrle, Simon
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 0
|e Corresponding author
245 _ _ |a Backchannels in conversations between autistic adults are less frequent and less diverse prosodically and lexically
260 _ _ |a Cambridge
|c 2024
|b Cambridge Univ. Press
336 7 _ |a article
|2 DRIVER
336 7 _ |a Output Types/Journal article
|2 DataCite
336 7 _ |a Journal Article
|b journal
|m journal
|0 PUB:(DE-HGF)16
|s 1737373803_8877
|2 PUB:(DE-HGF)
336 7 _ |a ARTICLE
|2 BibTeX
336 7 _ |a JOURNAL_ARTICLE
|2 ORCID
336 7 _ |a Journal Article
|0 0
|2 EndNote
520 _ _ |a Backchannels (BCs; listener signals such as ‘mmhm’ or ‘okay’) are a ubiquitous and essentialfeature of spoken interaction. They are used by listeners predominantly to support theongoing turn of their interlocutor and to signal understanding and agreement. Listenersseem to be highly sensitive to the exact realisations of BCs and to judge deviations fromtypical forms as negative. Very little is known about the use of BCs by speakers diagnosedwith autism spectrum disorder (ASD). In dialogue recordings of 28 German adults in twogroups of disposition-matched dyads (i.e., both interlocutors were either autistic or nonautistic),we found that the ASD group was characterised by (1) a lower rate of BCs perminute (particularly in the early stages of conversation), (2) less diversity in the lexicalrealisation of BCs and (3) a less diverse and flexible mapping of different intonation contoursto different BC types. We interpret these results as reflecting more general characteristics ofautistic as compared to non-autistic individuals, namely different strategies in signallingattention towards an interlocutor and less flexible behaviour in social interaction.
536 _ _ |a 5251 - Multilevel Brain Organization and Variability (POF4-525)
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5251
|c POF4-525
|f POF IV
|x 0
536 _ _ |a DFG project 281511265 - SFB 1252: Prominenz in Sprache (281511265)
|0 G:(GEPRIS)281511265
|c 281511265
|x 1
588 _ _ |a Dataset connected to CrossRef, Journals: juser.fz-juelich.de
700 1 _ |a Vogeley, Kai
|0 P:(DE-Juel1)176404
|b 1
700 1 _ |a Grice, Martine
|0 0000-0003-4973-4059
|b 2
773 _ _ |a 10.1017/langcog.2023.21
|g Vol. 16, no. 1, p. 108 - 133
|0 PERI:(DE-600)2499690-7
|n 1
|p 108 - 133
|t Language and cognition
|v 16
|y 2024
|x 1866-9808
856 4 _ |u https://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/1037612/files/PDF.pdf
|y OpenAccess
909 C O |o oai:juser.fz-juelich.de:1037612
|p openaire
|p open_access
|p VDB
|p driver
|p dnbdelivery
910 1 _ |a Forschungszentrum Jülich
|0 I:(DE-588b)5008462-8
|k FZJ
|b 1
|6 P:(DE-Juel1)176404
913 1 _ |a DE-HGF
|b Key Technologies
|l Natural, Artificial and Cognitive Information Processing
|1 G:(DE-HGF)POF4-520
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF4-525
|3 G:(DE-HGF)POF4
|2 G:(DE-HGF)POF4-500
|4 G:(DE-HGF)POF
|v Decoding Brain Organization and Dysfunction
|9 G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5251
|x 0
914 1 _ |y 2024
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0200
|2 StatID
|b SCOPUS
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0160
|2 StatID
|b Essential Science Indicators
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0130
|2 StatID
|b Social Sciences Citation Index
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a Creative Commons Attribution CC BY 4.0
|0 LIC:(DE-HGF)CCBY4
|2 HGFVOC
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)1170
|2 StatID
|b Current Contents - Arts and Humanities
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a JCR
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0100
|2 StatID
|b LANG COGN : 2022
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)1180
|2 StatID
|b Current Contents - Social and Behavioral Sciences
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0120
|2 StatID
|b Arts and Humanities Citation Index
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0501
|2 StatID
|b DOAJ Seal
|d 2024-03-15T13:07:36Z
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0500
|2 StatID
|b DOAJ
|d 2024-03-15T13:07:36Z
915 _ _ |a Fees
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0700
|2 StatID
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a IF < 5
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)9900
|2 StatID
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a OpenAccess
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0510
|2 StatID
915 _ _ |a Peer Review
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0030
|2 StatID
|b DOAJ : Double anonymous peer review
|d 2024-03-15T13:07:36Z
915 _ _ |a Article Processing Charges
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0561
|2 StatID
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a National-Konsortium
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0430
|2 StatID
|d 2024-12-11
|w ger
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0300
|2 StatID
|b Medline
|d 2024-12-11
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0199
|2 StatID
|b Clarivate Analytics Master Journal List
|d 2024-12-11
920 _ _ |l yes
920 1 _ |0 I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
|k INM-3
|l Kognitive Neurowissenschaften
|x 0
980 _ _ |a journal
980 _ _ |a VDB
980 _ _ |a UNRESTRICTED
980 _ _ |a I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
980 1 _ |a FullTexts


LibraryCollectionCLSMajorCLSMinorLanguageAuthor
Marc 21