001     139290
005     20210129212538.0
024 7 _ |a 10.1111/cdev.12178
|2 doi
024 7 _ |a 1467-8624
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a 0009-3920
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a WOS:000335786600020
|2 WOS
024 7 _ |a altmetric:1842724
|2 altmetric
024 7 _ |a pmid:24138135
|2 pmid
037 _ _ |a FZJ-2013-05289
082 _ _ |a 150
100 1 _ |a Köymen, Bahar
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 0
|e Corresponding author
245 _ _ |a Children's Norm Enforcement in Their Interactions With Peers
260 _ _ |a Malden, Mass.
|c 2013
|b Wiley-Blackwell
336 7 _ |a Journal Article
|b journal
|m journal
|0 PUB:(DE-HGF)16
|s 1405343925_22672
|2 PUB:(DE-HGF)
336 7 _ |a Output Types/Journal article
|2 DataCite
336 7 _ |a Journal Article
|0 0
|2 EndNote
336 7 _ |a ARTICLE
|2 BibTeX
336 7 _ |a JOURNAL_ARTICLE
|2 ORCID
336 7 _ |a article
|2 DRIVER
520 _ _ |a This study investigates how children negotiate social norms with peers. In Study 1, 48 pairs of 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 96) and in Study 2, 48 pairs of 5- and 7-year-olds (N = 96) were presented with sorting tasks with conflicting instructions (one child by color, the other by shape) or identical instructions. Three-year-olds differed from older children: They were less selective for the contexts in which they enforced norms, and they (as well as the older children to a lesser extent) used grammatical constructions objectifying the norms (“It works like this” rather than “You must do it like this”). These results suggested that children's understanding of social norms becomes more flexible during the preschool years.
536 _ _ |a 333 - Pathophysiological Mechanisms of Neurological and Psychiatric Diseases (POF2-333)
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-333
|c POF2-333
|f POF II
|x 0
536 _ _ |a 89572 - (Dys-)function and Plasticity (POF2-89572)
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-89572
|c POF2-89572
|x 1
|f POF II T
588 _ _ |a Dataset connected to CrossRef, juser.fz-juelich.de
700 1 _ |a Lieven, Elena
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 1
700 1 _ |a Engemann, Denis
|0 P:(DE-Juel1)144558
|b 2
|u fzj
700 1 _ |a Rakoczy, Hannes
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 3
700 1 _ |a Warneken, Felix
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 4
700 1 _ |a Tomasello, Michael
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 5
773 _ _ |a 10.1111/cdev.12178
|g p. n/a - n/a
|0 PERI:(DE-600)2047406-4
|n 3
|p 1108–1122
|t Child development
|v 85
|y 2013
|x 0009-3920
909 C O |o oai:juser.fz-juelich.de:139290
|p VDB
910 1 _ |a Forschungszentrum Jülich GmbH
|0 I:(DE-588b)5008462-8
|k FZJ
|b 2
|6 P:(DE-Juel1)144558
913 2 _ |a DE-HGF
|b Key Technologies
|l Decoding the Human Brain
|1 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-570
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-572
|2 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-500
|v (Dys-)function and Plasticity
|x 0
913 1 _ |a DE-HGF
|b Gesundheit
|l Funktion und Dysfunktion des Nervensystems
|1 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-330
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-333
|2 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-300
|v Pathophysiological Mechanisms of Neurological and Psychiatric Diseases
|x 0
|4 G:(DE-HGF)POF
|3 G:(DE-HGF)POF2
913 1 _ |a DE-HGF
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-89572
|v (Dys-)function and Plasticity
|x 1
|4 G:(DE-HGF)POF
|1 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-890
|3 G:(DE-HGF)POF3
|2 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-800
|b Programmungebundene Forschung
|l ohne Programm
914 1 _ |y 2013
915 _ _ |a Peer review unknown
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0040
|2 StatID
915 _ _ |a JCR
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0100
|2 StatID
915 _ _ |a WoS
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0130
|2 StatID
|b Social Sciences Citation Index
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0150
|2 StatID
|b Web of Science Core Collection
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0199
|2 StatID
|b Thomson Reuters Master Journal List
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0200
|2 StatID
|b SCOPUS
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0300
|2 StatID
|b Medline
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0310
|2 StatID
|b NCBI Molecular Biology Database
915 _ _ |a National-Konsortium
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0430
|2 StatID
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)1020
|2 StatID
|b Current Contents - Social and Behavioral Sciences
920 1 _ |0 I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
|k INM-3
|l Kognitive Neurowissenschaften
|x 0
980 _ _ |a journal
980 _ _ |a VDB
980 _ _ |a I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
980 _ _ |a UNRESTRICTED


LibraryCollectionCLSMajorCLSMinorLanguageAuthor
Marc 21