% IMPORTANT: The following is UTF-8 encoded. This means that in the presence
% of non-ASCII characters, it will not work with BibTeX 0.99 or older.
% Instead, you should use an up-to-date BibTeX implementation like “bibtex8” or
% “biber”.
@ARTICLE{Weidner:150577,
author = {Weidner, Ralph and Plewan, T. and Chen, Q. and Buchner, A.
and Weiss-Blankenhorn, Peter and Fink, G. R.},
title = {{T}he {M}oon {I}llusion and {S}ize-{D}istance {S}caling -
{E}vidence for {S}hared {N}eural {P}atterns.},
journal = {Journal of cognitive neuroscience},
volume = {26},
number = {8},
issn = {1530-8898},
address = {Cambridge, Mass.},
publisher = {MIT Pr. Journals},
reportid = {FZJ-2014-00626},
pages = {1871-1882},
year = {2014},
abstract = {A moon near to the horizon is perceived larger than a moon
at the zenith, although—obviously—the moon does not
change its size. In this study, the neural mechanisms
underlying the “moon illusion” were investigated using a
virtual 3-D environment and fMRI. Illusory perception of an
increased moon size was associated with increased neural
activity in ventral visual pathway areas including the
lingual and fusiform gyri. The functional role of these
areas was further explored in a second experiment. Left V3v
was found to be involved in integrating retinal size and
distance information, thus indicating that the brain regions
that dynamically integrate retinal size and distance play a
key role in generating the moon illusion.},
cin = {INM-3},
ddc = {400},
cid = {I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406},
pnm = {333 - Pathophysiological Mechanisms of Neurological and
Psychiatric Diseases (POF2-333) / 89572 - (Dys-)function and
Plasticity (POF2-89572)},
pid = {G:(DE-HGF)POF2-333 / G:(DE-HGF)POF2-89572},
typ = {PUB:(DE-HGF)16},
UT = {WOS:000338194800023},
pubmed = {pmid:24564459},
doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_00590},
url = {https://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/150577},
}