Home > Publications database > Recalibration of the auditory continuity illusion: Sensory and decisional effects > print |
001 | 19851 | ||
005 | 20210129210735.0 | ||
024 | 7 | _ | |2 pmid |a pmid:21276844 |
024 | 7 | _ | |2 pmc |a pmc:PMC3360525 |
024 | 7 | _ | |2 DOI |a 10.1016/j.heares.2011.01.013 |
024 | 7 | _ | |2 WOS |a WOS:000293726600018 |
037 | _ | _ | |a PreJuSER-19851 |
041 | _ | _ | |a eng |
082 | _ | _ | |a 570 |
084 | _ | _ | |2 WoS |a Neurosciences |
084 | _ | _ | |2 WoS |a Otorhinolaryngology |
100 | 1 | _ | |0 P:(DE-HGF)0 |a Riecke, L. |b 0 |
245 | _ | _ | |a Recalibration of the auditory continuity illusion: Sensory and decisional effects |
260 | _ | _ | |a Amsterdam [u.a.] |b Elsevier Science |c 2011 |
300 | _ | _ | |a 152–162 |
336 | 7 | _ | |a Journal Article |0 PUB:(DE-HGF)16 |2 PUB:(DE-HGF) |
336 | 7 | _ | |a Output Types/Journal article |2 DataCite |
336 | 7 | _ | |a Journal Article |0 0 |2 EndNote |
336 | 7 | _ | |a ARTICLE |2 BibTeX |
336 | 7 | _ | |a JOURNAL_ARTICLE |2 ORCID |
336 | 7 | _ | |a article |2 DRIVER |
440 | _ | 0 | |0 25405 |a Hearing Research |v 277 |y 1 |
500 | _ | _ | |a This work was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) Cognitie programma Grant 05104020. The authors thank Andrew Oxenham for useful discussions. Author CM is supported by an NIH grant (R01 DC007657). |
520 | _ | _ | |a An interrupted sound can be perceived as continuous when noise masks the interruption, creating an illusion of continuity. Recent findings have shown that adaptor sounds preceding an ambiguous target sound can influence listeners' rating of target continuity. However, it remains unclear whether these aftereffects on perceived continuity influence sensory processes, decisional processes (i.e., criterion shifts), or both. The present study addressed this question. Results show that the target sound was more likely to be rated as 'continuous' when preceded by adaptors that were perceived as clearly discontinuous than when it was preceded by adaptors that were heard (illusorily or veridically) as continuous. Detection-theory analyses indicated that these contrastive aftereffects reflect a combination of sensory and decisional processes. The contrastive sensory aftereffect persisted even when adaptors and targets were presented to opposite ears, suggesting a neural origin in structures that receive binaural inputs. Finally, physically identical but perceptually ambiguous adaptors that were rated as 'continuous' induced more reports of target continuity than adaptors that were rated as 'discontinuous'. This assimilative aftereffect was purely decisional. These findings confirm that judgments of auditory continuity can be influenced by preceding events, and reveal that these aftereffects have both sensory and decisional components. |
536 | _ | _ | |0 G:(DE-Juel1)FUEK409 |2 G:(DE-HGF) |x 0 |c FUEK409 |a Funktion und Dysfunktion des Nervensystems (FUEK409) |
536 | _ | _ | |0 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-89571 |a 89571 - Connectivity and Activity (POF2-89571) |c POF2-89571 |f POF II T |x 1 |
588 | _ | _ | |a Dataset connected to Web of Science, Pubmed |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Acoustic Stimulation |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Adaptation, Physiological |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Adaptation, Psychological |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Adult |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Audiometry |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Auditory Pathways: physiology |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Auditory Perception |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Auditory Threshold |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Cues |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Decision Theory |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Female |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Humans |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Illusions |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Male |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Middle Aged |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Noise: adverse effects |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Perceptual Masking |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Psychoacoustics |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Time Factors |
650 | _ | 2 | |2 MeSH |a Young Adult |
650 | _ | 7 | |2 WoSType |a J |
700 | 1 | _ | |0 P:(DE-HGF)0 |a Micheyl, C. |b 1 |
700 | 1 | _ | |0 P:(DE-HGF)0 |a Vanbussel, M. |b 2 |
700 | 1 | _ | |0 P:(DE-Juel1)VDB104568 |a Schreiner, C.S. |b 3 |u FZJ |
700 | 1 | _ | |0 P:(DE-HGF)0 |a Mendelsohn, D. |b 4 |
700 | 1 | _ | |0 P:(DE-HGF)0 |a Formisano, E. |b 5 |
773 | _ | _ | |0 PERI:(DE-600)2006374-X |a 10.1016/j.heares.2011.01.013 |g Vol. 277 |p 152–162 |q 277 |t Hearing research |v 277 |x 0378-5955 |y 2011 |
856 | 7 | _ | |2 Pubmed Central |u http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3360525 |
909 | C | O | |o oai:juser.fz-juelich.de:19851 |p VDB |
913 | 2 | _ | |0 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-571 |1 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-570 |2 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-500 |a DE-HGF |b Key Technologies |l Decoding the Human Brain |v Connectivity and Activity |x 0 |
913 | 1 | _ | |0 G:(DE-HGF)POF2-89571 |a DE-HGF |v Connectivity and Activity |x 1 |4 G:(DE-HGF)POF |1 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-890 |3 G:(DE-HGF)POF3 |2 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-800 |b Programmungebundene Forschung |l ohne Programm |
914 | 1 | _ | |y 2011 |
915 | _ | _ | |0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0040 |2 StatID |a Peer review unknown |
915 | _ | _ | |0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0100 |2 StatID |a JCR |
915 | _ | _ | |0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0111 |2 StatID |a WoS |b Science Citation Index Expanded |
915 | _ | _ | |0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0150 |2 StatID |a DBCoverage |b Web of Science Core Collection |
915 | _ | _ | |0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0199 |2 StatID |a DBCoverage |b Thomson Reuters Master Journal List |
915 | _ | _ | |0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0300 |2 StatID |a DBCoverage |b Medline |
920 | 1 | _ | |0 I:(DE-Juel1)INM-2-20090406 |g INM |k INM-2 |l Molekulare Organisation des Gehirns |x 0 |
970 | _ | _ | |a VDB:(DE-Juel1)134865 |
980 | _ | _ | |a VDB |
980 | _ | _ | |a ConvertedRecord |
980 | _ | _ | |a journal |
980 | _ | _ | |a I:(DE-Juel1)INM-2-20090406 |
980 | _ | _ | |a UNRESTRICTED |
Library | Collection | CLSMajor | CLSMinor | Language | Author |
---|