001     835122
005     20210129230825.0
024 7 _ |a 10.1007/s10803-016-2940-0
|2 doi
024 7 _ |a 0021-9185
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a 0162-3257
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a 1573-3432
|2 ISSN
024 7 _ |a altmetric:12749300
|2 altmetric
024 7 _ |a pmid:27757736
|2 pmid
024 7 _ |a WOS:000474414900028
|2 WOS
037 _ _ |a FZJ-2017-04990
082 _ _ |a 150
100 1 _ |a Kuzmanovic, Bojana
|0 P:(DE-Juel1)144165
|b 0
|e Corresponding author
245 _ _ |a Brief Report: Reduced Optimism Bias in Self-Referential Belief Updating in High-Functioning Autism
260 _ _ |a Dordrecht [u.a.]
|c 2019
|b Springer Science + Business Media B.V.
336 7 _ |a article
|2 DRIVER
336 7 _ |a Output Types/Journal article
|2 DataCite
336 7 _ |a Journal Article
|b journal
|m journal
|0 PUB:(DE-HGF)16
|s 1563198438_12474
|2 PUB:(DE-HGF)
336 7 _ |a ARTICLE
|2 BibTeX
336 7 _ |a JOURNAL_ARTICLE
|2 ORCID
336 7 _ |a Journal Article
|0 0
|2 EndNote
520 _ _ |a Previous research has demonstrated irrational asymmetry in belief updating: people tend to take into account good news and neglect bad news. Contradicting formal learning principles, belief updates were on average larger after better-than-expected information than after worse-than-expected information. In the present study, typically developing subjects demonstrated this optimism bias in self-referential judgments. In contrast, adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (ASD) were significantly less biased when updating self-referential beliefs (each group n = 21, matched for age, gender and IQ). These findings indicate a weaker influence of self-enhancing motives on prospective judgments in ASD. Reduced susceptibility to emotional and motivational biases in reasoning in ASD could elucidate impairments of social cognition, but may also confer important cognitive benefits.
536 _ _ |a 572 - (Dys-)function and Plasticity (POF3-572)
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-572
|c POF3-572
|f POF III
|x 0
588 _ _ |a Dataset connected to CrossRef
700 1 _ |a Rigoux, Lionel
|0 P:(DE-HGF)0
|b 1
700 1 _ |a Vogeley, Kai
|0 P:(DE-Juel1)145756
|b 2
773 _ _ |a 10.1007/s10803-016-2940-0
|0 PERI:(DE-600)2016724-6
|n 7
|p 2990-2998
|t Journal of autism and developmental disorders
|v 49
|y 2019
|x 1573-3432
856 4 _ |u https://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/835122/files/Kuzmanovic2019_Article_BriefReportReducedOptimismBias.pdf
|y Restricted
856 4 _ |u https://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/835122/files/Kuzmanovic2019_Article_BriefReportReducedOptimismBias.pdf?subformat=pdfa
|x pdfa
|y Restricted
909 C O |o oai:juser.fz-juelich.de:835122
|p VDB
910 1 _ |a INM-3
|0 I:(DE-HGF)0
|b 2
|6 P:(DE-Juel1)145756
910 1 _ |a Forschungszentrum Jülich
|0 I:(DE-588b)5008462-8
|k FZJ
|b 2
|6 P:(DE-Juel1)145756
913 1 _ |a DE-HGF
|b Key Technologies
|l Decoding the Human Brain
|1 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-570
|0 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-572
|2 G:(DE-HGF)POF3-500
|v (Dys-)function and Plasticity
|x 0
|4 G:(DE-HGF)POF
|3 G:(DE-HGF)POF3
914 1 _ |y 2019
915 _ _ |a Nationallizenz
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0420
|2 StatID
915 _ _ |a JCR
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0100
|2 StatID
|b J AUTISM DEV DISORD : 2015
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0200
|2 StatID
|b SCOPUS
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0300
|2 StatID
|b Medline
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0310
|2 StatID
|b NCBI Molecular Biology Database
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0199
|2 StatID
|b Thomson Reuters Master Journal List
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0130
|2 StatID
|b Social Sciences Citation Index
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)1180
|2 StatID
|b Current Contents - Social and Behavioral Sciences
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)1050
|2 StatID
|b BIOSIS Previews
915 _ _ |a DBCoverage
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0600
|2 StatID
|b Ebsco Academic Search
915 _ _ |a Peer Review
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)0030
|2 StatID
|b ASC
915 _ _ |a IF < 5
|0 StatID:(DE-HGF)9900
|2 StatID
920 _ _ |l yes
920 1 _ |0 I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
|k INM-3
|l Kognitive Neurowissenschaften
|x 0
980 _ _ |a journal
980 _ _ |a VDB
980 _ _ |a I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
980 _ _ |a UNRESTRICTED


LibraryCollectionCLSMajorCLSMinorLanguageAuthor
Marc 21