000856121 001__ 856121
000856121 005__ 20210129235240.0
000856121 0247_ $$2doi$$a10.1016/j.bandc.2018.09.007
000856121 0247_ $$2ISSN$$a0278-2626
000856121 0247_ $$2ISSN$$a1090-2147
000856121 0247_ $$2Handle$$a2128/21858
000856121 0247_ $$2pmid$$apmid:30290974
000856121 0247_ $$2WOS$$aWOS:000462806500009
000856121 037__ $$aFZJ-2018-05765
000856121 082__ $$a610
000856121 1001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)131693$$aLangner, Robert$$b0$$eCorresponding author
000856121 245__ $$aA network view on brain regions involved in experts’ object and pattern recognition: Implications for the neural mechanisms of skilled visual perception
000856121 260__ $$aAmsterdam [u.a.]$$bElsevier$$c2019
000856121 3367_ $$2DRIVER$$aarticle
000856121 3367_ $$2DataCite$$aOutput Types/Journal article
000856121 3367_ $$0PUB:(DE-HGF)16$$2PUB:(DE-HGF)$$aJournal Article$$bjournal$$mjournal$$s1552656490_21967
000856121 3367_ $$2BibTeX$$aARTICLE
000856121 3367_ $$2ORCID$$aJOURNAL_ARTICLE
000856121 3367_ $$00$$2EndNote$$aJournal Article
000856121 520__ $$aSkilled visual object and pattern recognition form the basis of many everyday behaviours. The game of chess has often been used as a model case for studying how long-term experience aides in perceiving objects and their spatio-functional interrelations. Earlier research revealed two brain regions, posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG) and collateral sulcus (CoS), to be linked to chess experts' superior object and pattern recognition, respectively. Here we elucidated the brain networks these two expertise-related regions are embedded in, employing resting-state functional connectivity analysis and meta-analytic connectivity modelling with the BrainMap database. pMTG was preferentially connected with dorsal visual stream areas and a parieto-prefrontal network for action planning, while CoS was preferentially connected with posterior medial cortex and hippocampus, linked to scene perception, perspective-taking and navigation. Functional profiling using BrainMap meta-data revealed that pMTG was linked to semantic processing as well as inhibition and attention, while CoS was linked to face and shape perception as well as passive viewing. Our findings suggest that pMTG subserves skilled object recognition by mediating the link between object identity and object affordances, while CoS subserves skilled pattern recognition by linking the position of individual objects with typical spatio-functional layouts of their environment stored in memory.
000856121 536__ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF3-571$$a571 - Connectivity and Activity (POF3-571)$$cPOF3-571$$fPOF III$$x0
000856121 536__ $$0G:(DE-Juel1)HGF-SMHB-2013-2017$$aSMHB - Supercomputing and Modelling for the Human Brain (HGF-SMHB-2013-2017)$$cHGF-SMHB-2013-2017$$fSMHB$$x1
000856121 588__ $$aDataset connected to CrossRef
000856121 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)131678$$aEickhoff, Simon$$b1$$ufzj
000856121 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aBilalić, Merim$$b2
000856121 773__ $$0PERI:(DE-600)1462261-0$$a10.1016/j.bandc.2018.09.007$$gp. S0278262618301453$$p74-86$$tBrain and cognition$$v131$$x0278-2626$$y2019
000856121 8564_ $$uhttps://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/856121/files/Langner_BRCG-2018_Brain%20Networks%20for%20Skilled%20Perception.pdf$$yPublished on 2018-10-02. Available in OpenAccess from 2019-10-02.
000856121 909CO $$ooai:juser.fz-juelich.de:856121$$pdnbdelivery$$pdriver$$pVDB$$popen_access$$popenaire
000856121 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)131693$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b0$$kFZJ
000856121 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)131678$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b1$$kFZJ
000856121 9131_ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF3-571$$1G:(DE-HGF)POF3-570$$2G:(DE-HGF)POF3-500$$3G:(DE-HGF)POF3$$4G:(DE-HGF)POF$$aDE-HGF$$bKey Technologies$$lDecoding the Human Brain$$vConnectivity and Activity$$x0
000856121 9141_ $$y2019
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1030$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bCurrent Contents - Life Sciences
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0200$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bSCOPUS
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0130$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bSocial Sciences Citation Index
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0600$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bEbsco Academic Search
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0530$$2StatID$$aEmbargoed OpenAccess
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0100$$2StatID$$aJCR$$bBRAIN COGNITION : 2017
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1180$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bCurrent Contents - Social and Behavioral Sciences
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0150$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bWeb of Science Core Collection
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0110$$2StatID$$aWoS$$bScience Citation Index
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0111$$2StatID$$aWoS$$bScience Citation Index Expanded
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)9900$$2StatID$$aIF < 5
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0030$$2StatID$$aPeer Review$$bASC
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0310$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bNCBI Molecular Biology Database
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0300$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bMedline
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0420$$2StatID$$aNationallizenz
000856121 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0199$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bClarivate Analytics Master Journal List
000856121 920__ $$lyes
000856121 9201_ $$0I:(DE-Juel1)INM-7-20090406$$kINM-7$$lGehirn & Verhalten$$x0
000856121 980__ $$ajournal
000856121 980__ $$aVDB
000856121 980__ $$aUNRESTRICTED
000856121 980__ $$aI:(DE-Juel1)INM-7-20090406
000856121 9801_ $$aFullTexts