000868235 001__ 868235
000868235 005__ 20220930130225.0
000868235 0247_ $$2doi$$a10.3389/fnhum.2019.00442
000868235 0247_ $$2Handle$$a2128/23717
000868235 0247_ $$2pmid$$apmid:31920600
000868235 0247_ $$2WOS$$aWOS:000504999600001
000868235 0247_ $$2altmetric$$aaltmetric:73119778
000868235 037__ $$aFZJ-2019-06796
000868235 082__ $$a610
000868235 1001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)177842$$aJording, Mathis$$b0$$eCorresponding author$$ufzj
000868235 245__ $$aDistinguishing Social From Private Intentions Through the Passive Observation of Gaze Cues
000868235 260__ $$aLausanne$$bFrontiers Research Foundation$$c2019
000868235 3367_ $$2DRIVER$$aarticle
000868235 3367_ $$2DataCite$$aOutput Types/Journal article
000868235 3367_ $$0PUB:(DE-HGF)16$$2PUB:(DE-HGF)$$aJournal Article$$bjournal$$mjournal$$s1576755969_22300
000868235 3367_ $$2BibTeX$$aARTICLE
000868235 3367_ $$2ORCID$$aJOURNAL_ARTICLE
000868235 3367_ $$00$$2EndNote$$aJournal Article
000868235 520__ $$aObserving others’ gaze is most informative during social encounters between humans: We can learn about potentially salient objects in the shared environment, infer others’ mental states and detect their communicative intentions. We almost automatically follow the gaze of others in order to check the relevance of the target of the other’s attention. This phenomenon called gaze cueing can be conceptualized as a triadic interaction involving a gaze initiator, a gaze follower and a gaze target, i.e., an object or person of interest in the environment. Gaze cueing can occur as “gaze pointing” with a communicative or “social” intention by the initiator, telling the observer that she/he is meant to follow, or as an incidental event, in which the observer follows spontaneously without any intention of the observed person. Here, we investigate which gaze cues let an observer ascribe a social intention to the observed person’s gaze and whether and to which degree previous eye contact in combination with an object fixation contributes to this ascription. We varied the orientation of the starting position of gaze toward the observer and the orientation of the end position of a lateral gaze shift. In two experiments participants had to infer from the gaze behavior either mere approach (“the person looked at me”) vs. a social (“the person wanted to show me something”) or a social vs. a private motivation (“the person was interested in something”). Participants differentially attributed either approach behavior, a social, or a private intention to the agent solely based on the passive observation of the two specific gaze cues of start and end position. While for the attribution of privately motivated behavior, participants relied solely on the end position of the gaze shift, the social interpretation of the observed behavior depended additionally upon initial eye contact. Implications of these results for future social gaze and social cognition research in general are discussed.
000868235 536__ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF3-572$$a572 - (Dys-)function and Plasticity (POF3-572)$$cPOF3-572$$fPOF III$$x0
000868235 588__ $$aDataset connected to CrossRef
000868235 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)144558$$aEngemann, Denis$$b1
000868235 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aEckert, Hannah$$b2
000868235 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aBente, Gary$$b3
000868235 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)176404$$aVogeley, Kai$$b4$$ufzj
000868235 773__ $$0PERI:(DE-600)2425477-0$$a10.3389/fnhum.2019.00442$$gVol. 13, p. 442$$p442$$tFrontiers in human neuroscience$$v13$$x1662-5161$$y2019
000868235 8564_ $$uhttps://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/868235/files/Jording_2019_Front%20Hum%20Neurosci_Distinguishing%20social%20from%20private%20intentions.pdf$$yOpenAccess
000868235 8564_ $$uhttps://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/868235/files/Jording_2019_Front%20Hum%20Neurosci_Distinguishing%20social%20from%20private%20intentions.pdf?subformat=pdfa$$xpdfa$$yOpenAccess
000868235 8767_ $$92020-01-20$$d2020-01-20$$eAPC$$jDeposit$$lDeposit: Frontiers$$zUSD 2507,50
000868235 909CO $$ooai:juser.fz-juelich.de:868235$$popenCost$$pVDB$$pdriver$$pOpenAPC$$popen_access$$popenaire$$pdnbdelivery
000868235 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)177842$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b0$$kFZJ
000868235 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)176404$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b4$$kFZJ
000868235 9131_ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF3-572$$1G:(DE-HGF)POF3-570$$2G:(DE-HGF)POF3-500$$3G:(DE-HGF)POF3$$4G:(DE-HGF)POF$$aDE-HGF$$bKey Technologies$$lDecoding the Human Brain$$v(Dys-)function and Plasticity$$x0
000868235 9141_ $$y2019
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0200$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bSCOPUS
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1050$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bBIOSIS Previews
000868235 915__ $$0LIC:(DE-HGF)CCBY4$$2HGFVOC$$aCreative Commons Attribution CC BY 4.0
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0100$$2StatID$$aJCR$$bFRONT HUM NEUROSCI : 2017
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0501$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bDOAJ Seal
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0500$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bDOAJ
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0111$$2StatID$$aWoS$$bScience Citation Index Expanded
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0150$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bWeb of Science Core Collection
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)9900$$2StatID$$aIF < 5
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0510$$2StatID$$aOpenAccess
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0030$$2StatID$$aPeer Review$$bDOAJ : Blind peer review
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0310$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bNCBI Molecular Biology Database
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0300$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bMedline
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0320$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bPubMed Central
000868235 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0199$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bClarivate Analytics Master Journal List
000868235 920__ $$lyes
000868235 9201_ $$0I:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406$$kINM-3$$lKognitive Neurowissenschaften$$x0
000868235 9801_ $$aFullTexts
000868235 980__ $$ajournal
000868235 980__ $$aVDB
000868235 980__ $$aUNRESTRICTED
000868235 980__ $$aI:(DE-Juel1)INM-3-20090406
000868235 980__ $$aAPC