000893928 001__ 893928
000893928 005__ 20210810182031.0
000893928 0247_ $$2doi$$a10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113418
000893928 0247_ $$2Handle$$a2128/28106
000893928 0247_ $$2pmid$$a34153427
000893928 0247_ $$2WOS$$aWOS:000674656800008
000893928 037__ $$aFZJ-2021-02938
000893928 082__ $$a610
000893928 1001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aJonen, M.$$b0
000893928 245__ $$aAdaption of a semantic picture-word interference paradigm for future language mapping with transcranial magnetic stimulation: A behavioural study
000893928 260__ $$aAmsterdam$$bElsevier$$c2021
000893928 3367_ $$2DRIVER$$aarticle
000893928 3367_ $$2DataCite$$aOutput Types/Journal article
000893928 3367_ $$0PUB:(DE-HGF)16$$2PUB:(DE-HGF)$$aJournal Article$$bjournal$$mjournal$$s1625988912_31105
000893928 3367_ $$2BibTeX$$aARTICLE
000893928 3367_ $$2ORCID$$aJOURNAL_ARTICLE
000893928 3367_ $$00$$2EndNote$$aJournal Article
000893928 520__ $$aNeuro-navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) helps to identify language-related cortical regions prior to brain tumour surgery. We adapted a semantic picture-word interference (PWI) paradigm from psycholinguistics to high-resolution TMS language mapping which prospectively can be used to specifically address the level of semantic processing. In PWI, pictures are presented along with distractor words which facilitate or inhibit the lexical access to the picture name. These modulatory effects of distractors can be annihilated in language-sensitive areas by the inhibitory effects of TMS on language processing. The rationale here is to observe the distractor effect without active stimulation and then to observe presumably its elimination by interference of the TMS stimulation. The special requirements to use PWI in this setting are (1) identifying word material for accelerating reliably naming latencies, choosing (2) the ideal presentation modality, and (3) the appropriate timing of distractor presentation. These are then controlled in real TMS language mapping. To adapt a semantic PWI naming paradigm for TMS application we employed 30 object-pictures in spoken German language. Part-whole associative semantic related or unrelated distractors were presented in two experiments including 15 healthy volunteers each, once auditorily and once visually. Data analysis across the entire stimulus set revealed a trend for facilitation in the visual condition, whereas no effects were observed for auditory distractors. In a sub-set, we found a significant facilitation effect for visual semantic distractors. Thus, with this study we provide a well-controlled item set for future studies implementing effective TMS language mapping applying visual semantic PWI. 
000893928 536__ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5251$$a5251 - Multilevel Brain Organization and Variability (POF4-525)$$cPOF4-525$$fPOF IV$$x0
000893928 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)131644$$aHeim, Stefan$$b1
000893928 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aGrünert, M.$$b2
000893928 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aNeuloh, G.$$b3
000893928 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aSakreida, K.$$b4$$eCorresponding author
000893928 773__ $$0PERI:(DE-600)2013604-3$$a10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113418$$p113418$$tBehavioural brain research$$v412$$x0166-4328$$y2021
000893928 8564_ $$uhttps://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/893928/files/Jonen%20et%20al_Revision_Manuscript_CLEANAcceptedChanges.pdf$$yPublished on 2021-06-18. Available in OpenAccess from 2022-12-18.
000893928 909CO $$ooai:juser.fz-juelich.de:893928$$pdnbdelivery$$pdriver$$pVDB$$popen_access$$popenaire
000893928 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)131644$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b1$$kFZJ
000893928 9131_ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF4-525$$1G:(DE-HGF)POF4-520$$2G:(DE-HGF)POF4-500$$3G:(DE-HGF)POF4$$4G:(DE-HGF)POF$$9G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5251$$aDE-HGF$$bKey Technologies$$lNatural, Artificial and Cognitive Information Processing$$vDecoding Brain Organization and Dysfunction$$x0
000893928 9141_ $$y2021
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0200$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bSCOPUS$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0160$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bEssential Science Indicators$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1050$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bBIOSIS Previews$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1190$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bBiological Abstracts$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0600$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bEbsco Academic Search$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0LIC:(DE-HGF)CCBYNCND4$$2HGFVOC$$aCreative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs CC BY-NC-ND 4.0
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0530$$2StatID$$aEmbargoed OpenAccess
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0100$$2StatID$$aJCR$$bBEHAV BRAIN RES : 2019$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1030$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bCurrent Contents - Life Sciences$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0113$$2StatID$$aWoS$$bScience Citation Index Expanded$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0150$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bWeb of Science Core Collection$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)9900$$2StatID$$aIF < 5$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0030$$2StatID$$aPeer Review$$bASC$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0300$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bMedline$$d2021-02-04
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0420$$2StatID$$aNationallizenz$$d2021-02-04$$wger
000893928 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0199$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bClarivate Analytics Master Journal List$$d2021-02-04
000893928 9201_ $$0I:(DE-Juel1)INM-1-20090406$$kINM-1$$lStrukturelle und funktionelle Organisation des Gehirns$$x0
000893928 9801_ $$aFullTexts
000893928 980__ $$ajournal
000893928 980__ $$aVDB
000893928 980__ $$aUNRESTRICTED
000893928 980__ $$aI:(DE-Juel1)INM-1-20090406