000902351 001__ 902351
000902351 005__ 20220103172052.0
000902351 0247_ $$2doi$$a10.3389/fnbeh.2021.749041
000902351 0247_ $$2Handle$$a2128/28997
000902351 0247_ $$2altmetric$$aaltmetric:114289585
000902351 0247_ $$2pmid$$a34658808
000902351 0247_ $$2WOS$$aWOS:000721598300001
000902351 037__ $$aFZJ-2021-04200
000902351 082__ $$a610
000902351 1001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aBoccadoro, Sara$$b0$$eCorresponding author
000902351 245__ $$aProactive vs. Reactive Aggression Within Two Modified Versions of the Taylor Aggression Paradigm
000902351 260__ $$aLausanne$$bFrontiers Research Foundation$$c2021
000902351 3367_ $$2DRIVER$$aarticle
000902351 3367_ $$2DataCite$$aOutput Types/Journal article
000902351 3367_ $$0PUB:(DE-HGF)16$$2PUB:(DE-HGF)$$aJournal Article$$bjournal$$mjournal$$s1636623098_7892
000902351 3367_ $$2BibTeX$$aARTICLE
000902351 3367_ $$2ORCID$$aJOURNAL_ARTICLE
000902351 3367_ $$00$$2EndNote$$aJournal Article
000902351 520__ $$aThe Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP) has been widely used to measure reactive aggression following provocation during competitive interactions. Besides being reactive, aggression can be goal-directed (proactive aggression). Our study presents a novel paradigm to investigate proactive aggression during competitive interactions. Sixty-seven healthy participants competed in two modified versions of the TAP against an ostensible opponent while skin conductance responses (SCRs) were recorded. During the proactive TAP (pTAP), only the participant could interfere with the ostensible opponent’s performance by blurring the screen. In the reactive TAP (rTAP), the opponent repeatedly provoked the participant by blurring the screen of the participant, impeding their chance to win. In both versions, the blurriness levels chosen by the participant served as a measure of aggression (unprovoked in the pTAP and provoked in the rTAP). In the pTAP, trial-by-trial mixed model analyses revealed higher aggression with higher self-reported selfishness. SCRs decreased with increasing proactive aggression. An interaction effect between gender and proactive aggression for the SCRs revealed increased SCRs at higher aggression levels in females, but lower SCRs at higher aggression levels in males. In the rTAP, SCRs were not associated with reactive aggression but aggression increased with increasing provocation and especially after losing against the opponent when provoked. While males showed higher aggression levels than females when unprovoked, reactive aggression increased more strongly in females with higher provocation. Mean levels of aggression in both tasks showed a high positive correlation. Our results highlight that, despite being intercorrelated and both motivated by selfishness, proactive and reactive aggression are differentially influenced by gender and physiological arousal. Proactive aggression is related to lower physiological arousal, especially in males, with females showing the opposite association. Reactive aggressive behavior is a result of individual responses to provocation, to which females seem to be more sensitive.
000902351 536__ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5253$$a5253 - Neuroimaging (POF4-525)$$cPOF4-525$$fPOF IV$$x0
000902351 588__ $$aDataset connected to CrossRef, Journals: juser.fz-juelich.de
000902351 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)172866$$aWagels, Lisa$$b1
000902351 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aHenn, Alina Theresa$$b2
000902351 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)179395$$aHüpen, Philippa$$b3
000902351 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aGraben, Lia$$b4
000902351 7001_ $$0P:(DE-HGF)0$$aRaine, Adrian$$b5
000902351 7001_ $$0P:(DE-Juel1)131781$$aNeuner, Irene$$b6
000902351 773__ $$0PERI:(DE-600)2452960-6$$a10.3389/fnbeh.2021.749041$$gVol. 15, p. 749041$$p749041$$tFrontiers in behavioral neuroscience$$v15$$x1662-5153$$y2021
000902351 8564_ $$uhttps://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/902351/files/fnbeh-15-749041.pdf$$yOpenAccess
000902351 909CO $$ooai:juser.fz-juelich.de:902351$$pdnbdelivery$$pdriver$$pVDB$$popen_access$$popenaire
000902351 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)172866$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b1$$kFZJ
000902351 9101_ $$0I:(DE-588b)5008462-8$$6P:(DE-Juel1)131781$$aForschungszentrum Jülich$$b6$$kFZJ
000902351 9131_ $$0G:(DE-HGF)POF4-525$$1G:(DE-HGF)POF4-520$$2G:(DE-HGF)POF4-500$$3G:(DE-HGF)POF4$$4G:(DE-HGF)POF$$9G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5253$$aDE-HGF$$bKey Technologies$$lNatural, Artificial and Cognitive Information Processing$$vDecoding Brain Organization and Dysfunction$$x0
000902351 9141_ $$y2021
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0200$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bSCOPUS$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0160$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bEssential Science Indicators$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0LIC:(DE-HGF)CCBY4$$2HGFVOC$$aCreative Commons Attribution CC BY 4.0
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)1040$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bZoological Record$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0100$$2StatID$$aJCR$$bFRONT BEHAV NEUROSCI : 2019$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0501$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bDOAJ Seal$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0500$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bDOAJ$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0113$$2StatID$$aWoS$$bScience Citation Index Expanded$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0700$$2StatID$$aFees$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0150$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bWeb of Science Core Collection$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)9900$$2StatID$$aIF < 5$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0510$$2StatID$$aOpenAccess
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0030$$2StatID$$aPeer Review$$bDOAJ : Blind peer review$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0561$$2StatID$$aArticle Processing Charges$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0300$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bMedline$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0320$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bPubMed Central$$d2021-02-04
000902351 915__ $$0StatID:(DE-HGF)0199$$2StatID$$aDBCoverage$$bClarivate Analytics Master Journal List$$d2021-02-04
000902351 9201_ $$0I:(DE-Juel1)INM-4-20090406$$kINM-4$$lPhysik der Medizinischen Bildgebung$$x0
000902351 9201_ $$0I:(DE-Juel1)INM-10-20170113$$kINM-10$$lJara-Institut Brain structure-function relationships$$x1
000902351 9201_ $$0I:(DE-Juel1)VDB1046$$kJARA-BRAIN$$lJülich-Aachen Research Alliance - Translational Brain Medicine$$x2
000902351 980__ $$ajournal
000902351 980__ $$aVDB
000902351 980__ $$aUNRESTRICTED
000902351 980__ $$aI:(DE-Juel1)INM-4-20090406
000902351 980__ $$aI:(DE-Juel1)INM-10-20170113
000902351 980__ $$aI:(DE-Juel1)VDB1046
000902351 9801_ $$aFullTexts