% IMPORTANT: The following is UTF-8 encoded. This means that in the presence
% of non-ASCII characters, it will not work with BibTeX 0.99 or older.
% Instead, you should use an up-to-date BibTeX implementation like “bibtex8” or
% “biber”.
@ARTICLE{Rippon:904414,
author = {Rippon, Gina and Eliot, Lise and Genon, Sarah and Joel,
Daphna},
title = {{H}ow hype and hyperbole distort the neuroscience of sex
differences},
journal = {PLoS biology},
volume = {19},
number = {5},
issn = {1544-9173},
address = {Lawrence, KS},
publisher = {PLoS},
reportid = {FZJ-2021-05984},
pages = {e3001253 -},
year = {2021},
abstract = {Sex/gender differences in the human brain attract attention
far beyond the neuroscience community. Given the interest of
nonspecialists, it is important that researchers studying
human female–male brain difference assume greater
responsibility for the accurate communication of their
findings.Research into female–male brain differences is
believed important for understanding sex/gender disparities
in neurological and mental health, in educational and
occupational achievement, and for informing diversity and
inclusion initiatives. (We use “sex/gender” to embrace
the difficulty of disentangling participants’
“sex”—biological attributes including genitalia,
sex-related chromosomes, and hormones—and
“gender”—psychological and social attributes
associated with males and females—as distinct variables in
human neuroscience.) Interest in the outcomes of brain
sex/gender difference research extends beyond the research
specialty itself, calling for attention to issues of
transparency and clarity in communicating such
findings.Concerned researchers have warned about the ease
with which new and existing datasets can be mined for
male–female group differences, often leading to reporting
bias and false positives or failures to report effect sizes
where differences have been found [1–3]. Likewise,
they’ve raised concerns about the misuse of sex/gender
brain findings in the public sphere, where the data have
been translated for popular communication in careless and
stereotypical ways [3,4]. Much less attention has been paid
to problems of misrepresentation arising from the narrative
and interpretive context in scientific articles themselves.
Here, we highlight the need for “impression management”
in research reports on brain sex/gender difference.A
recurrent problem in such studies is that the qualitative
terminology used to describe the results does not accurately
reflect the actual findings. Contemporary brain imaging
research employs datasets with hundreds or thousands of
measures, which are analyzed using multiple comparisons.
Frequently, statistically significant differences are found
in only a small fraction of possible contrasts. But this is
rarely made clear in the abstract and discussion section,
even if it is acknowledged in the results. For example, in a
study of brain connectivity networks, the abstract states
that sex differences were “prominent … at multiple
scales of analysis” despite only $2\%$ of the thousands of
comparisons showing small statistical differences [5]. Such
hyperbole can be compounded when there is unjustified
emphasis on marginally significant findings and/or findings
that did not actually survive correction for multiple
comparisons. For example, the title of another recent paper
referred to sex differences in “brain growth
trajectories” even though none of the 46 critical measures
showed significant sex-by-age differences after correcting
for multiple comparisons [6]. The discussion further focused
on sex/gender differences that had not survived appropriate
statistical corrections.The preference for positive results
in scientific publications is an acknowledged problem
[1,2,4], but is no excuse for glossing over the larger
context of any statistically significant findings. To ensure
an accurate reflection of all statistical comparisons,
journal editors and reviewers should require that these be
reported by indexing the number of (genuine) statistically
significant differences to the total number of comparisons
made or as a ratio of differences to similarities. This
should be included in the abstract, results, and discussion
sections to help readers gauge the true degree of
group-level differences. Verbal summaries of the findings
should use terms such as “many,” “strong,” etc. only
when justified by this numerical index.A second set of
problems emerges when sex/gender comparisons are conducted
by investigators naïve to the field or as an “add-on”
to the main objective of the study. Such researchers
commonly adopt an essentialist binary framework and an
evolutionary perspective that biases the analysis, design,
and interpretation of results. The underlying assumption is
that female–male differences are determined by biological
factors (i.e., “sex”), ignoring the myriad of
psychosocial influences (i.e., “gender”) that can affect
the brain and may not have been assayed as possible
covariates or considered when interpreting the results. For
example, a paper on “social brains” [7] interpreted some
limited male-female differences in correlations between
certain brain structures and social variables as evidence
“that human survival has been optimized toward
sex-specific strategies to successfully navigate the social
world (7, p. 9).” Considering the interest of such
research to nonexperts, journal editors and reviewers should
ensure authors acknowledge the full biopsychosocial
complexity of sex/gender and avoid the impression that there
is a single, well-established, and noncontroversial
interpretation of their findings.This leads us to the third
issue: the ease of deriving a post hoc rationalization for
discovery-based findings of sex/gender brain differences.
The core of this problem is a failure to acknowledge that
the link between structure and function in the human brain
is not well defined. Most mental processes engage many
overlapping neural structures and circuits, so researchers
have a wide range of choices in the behavioral
interpretation they apply to any sex/gender difference in
structure or connectivity. This makes it all too easy to
retroactively spin a speculative relationship to some gender
disparity around any differences detected in large-scale
human neuroimaging databases, such as a high-profile article
that interpreted modest connectome sex/gender differences as
supporting “co-ordinated action” by male brains versus
“communication” by female brains [8]. Although an
important solution for this issue is the preregistration of
research protocols, where post hoc analyses are regulated
and selective inference is detected [9], impression
management in the final communication of research findings
can remain a problem.Editorial policy and reviewer
guidelines commonly focus on methodological issues and pay
less attention to the responsible use of language in the
final text, even though the title and abstract are often the
sole source of a study’s take-home message for
nonspecialists.In the interests of both science and society,
neuroscientists need to think carefully about how they
present findings about brain differences between socially
segregated groups of healthy humans. They need to recognize
that any neurobiological comparison between such groups
raises the potential for stereotyping and stigmatization.
That means ensuring that research design and methodology
reflect current understanding of sex/gender. It also means
paying careful attention to the impression given by
selective narrative framing and inaccurate use of
quantitative descriptors. A failure to clarify the practical
significance of complex statistical findings or to
acknowledge the multifaceted biopsychosocial contributions
to sex/gender groupings gives undue weight to the relevance
of the findings. Given the real costs of entrenched
sex/gender disparities across society, neuroscientists have
a duty to prevent the spread of misinformation about the
neural basis of such differences.},
cin = {INM-7},
ddc = {610},
cid = {I:(DE-Juel1)INM-7-20090406},
pnm = {5251 - Multilevel Brain Organization and Variability
(POF4-525)},
pid = {G:(DE-HGF)POF4-5251},
typ = {PUB:(DE-HGF)16},
pubmed = {pmid:33970901},
UT = {WOS:000664233900002},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pbio.3001253},
url = {https://juser.fz-juelich.de/record/904414},
}